Saliha Şahintürk1, İrem Doğanoğlu1, Lütfü Hanoğlu2, Erol Yıldırım3

1İstanbul Medipol University, Research Institute For Health Sciences and Technologies (SABITA), İstanbul, Türkiye
2Department of Neurology, İstanbul Medipol University Faculty of Medicine, İstanbul, Türkiye
3Department of Psychology, İstanbul Medipol University, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, İstanbul, Türkiye

Keywords: Alzheimer disease, apraxia, dementia, mild cognitive impairment, neuropsychology

Abstract

Objectives: This study aimed to examine the differences in upper limb apraxia assessments and neuropsychological profiles of patients diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and healthy controls.

Patients and methods: A total of 53 participants were included in the retrospective study, including nine patients with MCI, 23 patients diagnosed with AD, and 21 healthy patients equivalent in age and education level. The participants’ data were collected between July 2021 and December 2022. A 12-question mini-test taken from the Test of Upper Limb Apraxia (TULIA) was used in the apraxia evaluation. Individuals’ upper limb apraxia evaluations were compared according to diagnostic groups, and their neuropsychological profiles were also examined.

Results: Apraxia was found to be associated with impairments in memory retrieval function, executive dysfunction, and decrease in object naming performance. Significant differences were observed between diagnostic groups in both apraxia assessment and neuropsychological tests.

Conclusion: The findings indicate that the cognitive profile that emerges with the combined use of upper extremity apraxia assessment and related neuropsychological tests may serve as a marker and guide in the planning and correct execution of treatment in the transition to Alzheimer-type dementia, similar to other neuropsychological tests.

Introduction

The term apraxia, used to describe problems in planning and executing movements resulting from neurological dysfunction, was first introduced by Steinthal in 1881.[1] Today, it is commonly defined as an impairment in the ability to move that does not arise from weakened motor performance due to weakness, sensory loss, ataxia, akinesia, bradykinesia, hypometria, tremor, dystonia, chorea, ballismus, athetosis, or myoclonus.[2] Apraxia has many classifications and specific types, including well-known forms such as ideational apraxia, conceptual apraxia, ideomotor apraxia, limb-kinetic apraxia, constructional apraxia, and conduction apraxia.[3] Limb apraxia refers to the impairment in the ability to perform learned skilled movements resulting from neurological damage, which cannot be explained by primary motor and sensory deficits, problems in understanding tasks, or object recognition disorders.[1,3,4-7] There are four main forms of upper limb apraxia: ideomotor, limb-kinetic, conceptual, and ideational apraxia.[8] The measurement tools recently used to assess apraxia are as follows: the Test of Upper Limb Apraxia (TULIA);[9] its short version, Apraxia Screen of TULIA (AST); DEKODa apraxia test;[10] Evaluation of Upper Limb Apraxia (EULA);[11] and Dementia Apraxia Test (DATE).[12] Test of Upper Limb Apraxia and AST have been adapted for Turkish.[13,14]

Limb apraxia can occur following neurological damage, stroke, or neurodegenerative diseases, and it can also manifest from the early stages of neurodegenerative diseases.[15] A substantial portion of the research on apraxia has been conducted on patients with stroke.[16] Although it is stated that apraxia can emerge in the early phases of neurodegenerative diseases, apraxia remains an underresearched area in neurodegenerative diseases compared to other cognitive functions.[17] Therefore, examining the presentation of apraxia in neurodegenerative diseases where cognitive function losses are observed is considered to be crucial.

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease that leads to a progressive impairment of cognitive functions with amnesia in the forefront, behavioral issues, and presenting as dementia with the loss of daily functionality in patients.[17] In addition to amnestic losses, other cognitive issues such as executive function disorders, agnosia, aphasia, and apraxia also emerge during the course of the disease.[18,19] While there are numerous studies on the cognitive losses accompanying AD, research on apraxia is limited.

The National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association (NIA-AA) updated the diagnostic criteria for AD in 2011, incorporating neuropsychological tests, advanced imaging methods, and cerebrospinal fluid analysis.[20] Apraxia, which has an insidious onset, gradually worsening over months or even years and accompanied by significant memory losses, was included in the diagnostic criteria established by the NIA-AA in 1981 and updated in 2011.[18,21]

In the literature, it has been observed that scores from apraxia screening tests, when compared with healthy controls, successfully detect the early stages of neurodegenerative diseases such as AD and that there are significant impairments in the patients' ability to mimic hand and finger postures.[21] Although some apraxia tests were originally designed for patients with stroke, they were also applied to patients with dementia, yielding reliable results for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD.[22] A limb and facial praxis tool developed to support the differential diagnosis of dementia demonstrated high diagnostic accuracy in detecting early-stage AD in elderly patients.[16]

Examining the relationship between neuropsychological test scores that play a crucial role in the diagnostic process and apraxia scores is vital in understanding how neuropsychological manifestations and apraxia scores differ according to diagnoses. As studies have shown, both neuropsychological tests and the mentioned apraxia tests aid the diagnostic process in pathological conditions accompanied by cognitive problems. However, studies exploring the relationship between apraxia and neuropsychological tests are limited.

In light of this information, studies that explore the relationship between AD, including memory and other cognitive function issues, and apraxia could contribute to the literature by clarifying this relationship. Hence, this study aimed to examine and reveal the differences in upper limb apraxia assessments and neuropsychological profiles of patients diagnosed with AD and MCI and healthy controls.

Material and Methods

The retrospective study utilized data from patients diagnosed with MCI and AD and healthy individuals who applied to the neurology outpatient clinic of the SABITA (Health Science and Technology Research Institute)-fiNCAN Laboratory and underwent neuropsychological assessments in the hospital’s neuropsychology laboratory between July 2021 and December 2022. The analysis of the collected data was conducted between January 2023 and February 2023. The MCI patients were the amnestic type. The study included nine patients with MCI, 23 patients with AD, and 21 healthy volunteers matched for age and education level, for a total of 53 participants. Participants with alcohol/ substance dependence or intellectual disability were not included in this study. The diagnosis of MCI was based on the criteria proposed by Petersen et al.,[23] whereas the AD diagnosis was made in accordance with the recommendations of the NIA-AA Workgroups[20] by the neuropsychological and clinical assessments of an expert neurologist. The data for the neuropsychological tests and apraxia assessments were collected by three psychologists.

Participants' demographic information was collected from the hospital's patient tracking system, while neuropsychological data were gathered from the neuropsychological test battery registered in the neuropsychology laboratory. The neuropsychological tests administered to participants and used in the study include the following: Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS)- Revised forward and backward digit span, category naming verbal fluency, and K-A-S verbal fluency, fruit-name counting, Boston Naming Test (BNT) spontaneous naming, Standardized Mini-Mental State Examination (SMMT), Geriatric Depression Scale, Beck Depression Inventory, WMS immediate and long-term visual memory, WMS immediate and long-term logical memory, Verbal Test of Memory Processes (VTMP)-Immediate Memory, VTMP-Total Learning, VTMP-Delayed Recall, and VTMP-Retention, Stroop test, clock drawing test, face recognition test, and the judgement of line orientation test.

In this study, the mini-test used for apraxia assessment consists of 12 questions taken from the TULIA, which was validated and shown to be reliable in Turkish.[13] The test was divided into three categories, each containing four questions covering nonsymbolic movements, symbolic movements, and imitation of object use. The reason for selecting these categories and questions is based on clinical observations, believing that they can effectively reflect upper limb apraxia during neuropsychological evaluation and provide a practical assessment. This tool lacked a cutoff value or normative table and was intended for practical clinical observation, and the total scores obtained were considered in the evaluation.

Statistical analysis

The required sample size was determined through power analysis with the G*Power version 3.1 (Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany), considering a one-way analysis of variance for three groups. The power analysis established that the current study required at least a total of 42 participants, with a power of 0.80, an alpha error value of 0.05, and an effect size of 0.5. Data from 53 individuals was used in the study.

Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS version 25.0 software (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). All data were reviewed for missing and outlier values, and descriptive statistics were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or frequency. The adherence of variables to normal distribution was assessed with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, and nonparametric tests were employed due to the absence of normal distribution. The Kruskal-Wallis test was used for variance analysis. Complementary comparison techniques were implemented to determine the groups with significant differences, and the Mann-Whitney U test was preferred for pairwise comparisons. Spearman correlation analysis was utilized to examine correlations.

Results

Apraxia scores statistically significantly differed according to diagnostic subcategories (χ2 =12.650, p=0.002). Significant statistical differences were found between the AD and MCI groups (p=0.027), as well as the AD and healthy control groups (p=0.001). Examining the rank means, it was observed that the AD group scored significantly lower than both the MCI and healthy control groups. No significant difference was observed between the MCI and healthy control groups (p=0.748, Table 1).

In the neuropsychological assessment of the sample, statistically significant differences were found according to the diagnosis variable in the following evaluations: backward digit span, category naming, K-A-S, fruit-name counting, BNT spontaneous naming, SMMT, WMS immediate visual, long-term visual, immediate logical, and long-term logical memory, VTMP-Immediate Memory, VTMP-Total Learning, VTMP-Delayed Recall, and VTMP-Retention, Stroop error score, clock drawing, and line orientation tests (p<0.05). The forward digit span, face recognition, Geriatric Depression Scale, and Beck Depression Inventory did not demonstrate differences based on the diagnostic variable (p>0.05, Table 2).

Significant differences (p<0.05) were found between the AD and MCI groups in backward digit span (p=0.008), K-A-S (p=0.022), fruit-name counting (p=0.001), BNT spontaneous naming (p=0.007), WMS immediate logical (p=0.002) and long-term logical (p=0.005) memory, VTMP-Total Learning (p=0.004), and VTMP-Delayed Recall (p=0.022). Furthermore, highly significant differences (p<0.001) were observed in clock drawing (p=0.000), SMMT (p=0.000), WMS immediate visual (p=0.000) and long-term visual (p=0.000) memory, and VTMP-Retention (p=0.000). No significant differences (p>0.05) were observed in category naming (p=0.067), VTMP-Immediate Memory (p=0.108), Stroop error score (p=0.061), face recognition (p=0.363), and line orientation (p=0.061). Across all tests, patients with AD scored lower than those with MCI.

Significant differences were found in neuropsychological test scores between AD patients and healthy controls in the backward digit span (p=0.002). Highly significant differences (p<0.001) were also found in K-A-S, fruit-name counting, BNT spontaneous naming, WMS immediate and long-term logical memory, VTMP-Total Learning, VTMP-Delayed Recall, clock drawing, SMMT, WMS immediate and long-term visual memory, and VTMP-Retention (all p=0.000). No significant difference (p>0.05) was observed in the face recognition test (p=0.942). Patients with AD scored lower than healthy controls across all tests in which significant differences were found.

Significant differences (p<0.05) were found in neuropsychological test scores between patients with MCI and healthy controls in the WMS long-term logical memory (p=0.018), VTMP-Immediate Memory (p=0.036), VTMPTotal Learning (p=0.002), and VTMP-Delayed Recall (p=0.002). No significant differences were observed in other tests (p>0.05). Patients with MCI scored lower than healthy controls in the tests where differences were observed.

The correlation between apraxia scores and neuropsychological test results was examined, and significant correlations (p<0.05) were found in verbal fluency-category naming, BNT spontaneous naming, WMS long-term logical memory, and VTMP-Total Learning. Furthermore, highly significant correlations (p<0.01) were observed in verbal fluency-K-A-S, fruit-name counting, SMMT, WMS immediate visual and long-term visual memory, VTMP-Delayed Recall, VTMP-Retention, Stroop error score, and clock drawing. The correlation between the Stroop error score and apraxia score was negative (Table 3).

Discussion

In this study, a comparison of upper limb apraxia assessments among individuals diagnosed with AD, MCI, and healthy controls was conducted, and the relationships between upper limb apraxia and neuropsychological tests were examined. The results demonstrated that apraxia scores significantly differed among the three groups. The difference was more pronounced between the AD and healthy control groups. However, no significant difference was observed between the MCI and healthy control groups. The results are consistent with other studies that revealed significant differences in apraxia assessments between AD, MCI, and healthy controls.[10,12,16,21,22,24,25]

Studies in the literature report that apraxia can be observed from the early stages of MCI and AD,[26] that one out of 10 patients with MCI and more than one out of three patients with AD present with apraxia, and that as the severity of dementia increases, the risk of apraxia rises.[22] Furthermore, results from tasks related to tool use suggest that both the sensorimotor knowledge required for tool manipulation and the semantic knowledge about the tool's function are impaired from the early stages of AD.[27] Mechanical knowledge, production systems, and topographic information may be preserved in the early and middle stages of AD.[28] Although various studies exist in this area, more research is needed on the apraxia profiles observed in AD and MCI.

The present study found significant differences in apraxia scores between the AD and MCI groups, as well as the AD and healthy control groups, while no significant difference was observed between patients with MCI and healthy controls. This suggests that apraxia could be a neuropsychological parameter that appears in the transition to Alzheimer-type dementia, or it may arise alongside the global deterioration observed in AD.

Analyses conducted to determine how neuropsychological evaluations differed according to diagnostic subgroups revealed significant differences in all tests, except for the digit span, face recognition, Geriatric Depression Scale, and Beck Depression Inventory. This differentiation was significant in all tests, except for category naming, VTMP-Immediate Memory, error score, face recognition, and line orientation tests, and highly significant for clock drawing, SMMT, WMS immediate visual and long-term visual memory, and VTMP-Retention scores between the AD and MCI groups. Neuropsychological test scores were significantly different in all tests between the AD and healthy control groups, except for the face recognition test. This differentiation was significant in the backward digit span and highly significant in all other tests. Significant differences were observed only in the WMS long-term logical, VTMP-Immediate Memory, VTMP-Total Learning, and VTMP-Delayed Recall scores between the MCI and healthy control groups.

A review of the literature reveals no recent study examining the neuropsychological profile accompanying upper limb apraxia, particularly in conjunction with other cognitive functions. Additionally, in a study involving patients with corticobasal degeneration (CBD), both with and without apraxia, no significant differences were found between the two groups in apraxia performance scores and executive functions.[29] Furthermore, no significant differences were observed between the two groups in other neuropsychological tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, trail making test, Stroop test, digit span, and verbal fluency tests. In our study, the correlation between apraxia scores and neuropsychological tests was examined. The findings are partially consistent with a study demonstrating that apraxia is associated with cognitive functions such as aphasia, memory, and mental slowing.[30] However, research in this area is limited, and more studies are required for more definitive interpretations.

In dementia, reduced gray matter volume at the right temporo-occipito-parietal junction is associated with problems in pantomiming.[31] The errors observed in pantomime tasks among dementia patients are predominantly movementorientation errors, which occur in conjunction with a decline in visuospatial performance.[15] Errors in praxis due to movement-orientation arise when the movement is not synchronized with the spatial position of the limb. However, in our study, no significant correlation was found between apraxia scores and the scores from the line orientation and face recognition tests. These findings contradict the information available in the literature.

When the correlation between the measured apraxia scores and other neuropsychological tests were examined, significant correlations were found in verbal fluency-category naming, BNT spontaneous naming, WMS long-term logical memory, and VTMP-Total Learning, with highly significant correlations in K-A-S verbal fluency, fruit-name counting, SMMT, WMS immediate visual and long-term visual memory, VTMP-Delayed Recall, VTMP-Retention, Stroop error score, and clock drawing. The correlation between the Stroop error score and apraxia score was negative. Patients with AD predominantly experience losses in memory, as well as in attention, executive functions, constructional skills, and performance in object naming and understanding, and these losses are associated with limb apraxia.[32] The losses in these cognitive areas are related to the ability to perform movements with the correct sequence, manipulation, and comprehension, as well as losses in action semantics or impairments in object naming if a tool-based praxis performance is to be demonstrated.

The limitations of the study include its retrospective nature, the absence of staging in AD patients, the limited sample size, and the failure to consider coexisting cerebrovascular diseases and intracranial lesions that could cause apraxia as exclusion criteria. In addition, the use of a mini-test derived from the TULIA test for measuring apraxia and the lack of neuroimaging findings are among the limitations. Future studies are recommended to establish more rigorous exclusion criteria, work with a larger sample group, include various measurement tools, and incorporate neuroimaging findings. Additionally, longitudinal studies examining the apraxia profile in AD alongside neuropsychological tests are considered important.

In conclusion, this study found apraxia to be associated with impairments in memory retrieval, executive function disorders, and a decline in object naming performance. Additionally, significant differences were observed in apraxia and neuropsychological tests in both of the patient groups compared to healthy controls. This suggests that while it may not be helpful in the early stages, the cognitive profile revealed through the combined use of upper limb apraxia assessments and related neuropsychological tests can serve as a marker and guide in distinguishing diagnoses, planning treatment, and guiding its proper execution similar to other neuropsychological tests. This study contributes to the literature by examining upper limb apraxia alongside the neuropsychological profiles of patients within the context of AD and MCI. Future studies examining apraxia together with neuropsychological tests in other types of dementia where cognitive functions are impaired are crucial in understanding impairments in praxis skills and the development of potential treatment methods.

Cite this article as: Şahintürk S, Doğanoğlu İ, Hanoğlu L, Yıldırım E. Investigation of the relationship between upper limb apraxia and neuropsychological profile in Alzheimer’s disease dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Turk J Neurol 2024;30(2):93-101. doi: 10.55697/tnd.2024.4.

Data Sharing Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Ethics Committee Approval

The study protocol was approved by the İstanbul Medipol University Non-Interventional Clinical Research Ethics Committee (date: 05.01.2023, no: 06). The study was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

Idea/concept, design, control/ supervision, data collection and/or processing, analysis and/or interpretation, literature review, writing the article, critical review, references and fundings, materials: S.Ş., İ.D.; Statistical analysis: L.H., E.Y.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

Financial Disclosure

The authors received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.

References

  1. Bartolo A, Ham HS. A cognitive overview of limb apraxia. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2016;16:75. doi: 10.1007/s11910-016-0675-0.
  2. Rothi LJ, Ochipa C, Heilman KM. A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1991;8:443-58.
  3. Osiurak F, Rossetti Y. Definition: Limb apraxia. Cortex 2017;93:228. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.010.
  4. Ochipa C, Gonzalez Rothi LJ. Limb apraxia. Semin Neurol 2000;20:471-8. doi: 10.1055/s-2000-13180.
  5. Niessen E, Fink GR, Weiss PH. Apraxia, pantomime and the parietal cortex. Neuroimage Clin 2014;5:42-52. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.017.
  6. Paulraj SR, Schendel K, Curran B, Dronkers NF, Baldo JV. Role of the left hemisphere in visuospatial working memory. J Neurolinguistics 2018;48:133-41. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2018.04.006.
  7. Park JE. Apraxia: Review and update. J Clin Neurol 2017;13:317-24. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2017.13.4.317.
  8. Heilman KM. Upper limb apraxia. Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2021;27:1602-1623. doi: 10.1212/ CON.0000000000001014.
  9. Vanbellingen T, Kersten B, Van de Winckel A, Bellion M, Baronti F, Müri R, et al. A new bedside test of gestures in stroke: the apraxia screen of TULIA (AST). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:389-92. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2010.213371.
  10. Evlice A, Kayserili G, Kurt P, Keskinoğlu P, Uçku R, Yener G. A new apraxia test for Turkish elderly; DEKODa. J Neurol Sci 2016;33:30-7.
  11. Perez-Marmol JM, Lopez-Alcalde S, Carnero-Pardo C, Canadas-De la Fuente GA, Peralta-Ramirez MI, GarciaRios MC. Creation and design of a test for the Evaluation of Upper Limb Apraxia (EULA) based on a cognitive model: a pilot study. Rev Neurol 2015;60:66-74.
  12. Yliranta A, Jehkonen M. Limb and face apraxias in frontotemporal dementia: A systematic scoping review. Cortex 2020;129:529-47. doi: 10.1016/j. cortex.2020.03.023.
  13. Çeğil T. TULIA’nın (Üst Ekstremite için Apraksi Testi) Türkçe standardizasyon, geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması [Yayınlanmamış Yüksek Lisans Tezi]. İstanbul: İstanbul Medipol Üniversitesi Sağlık Bilimleri Enstitüsü; 2019.
  14. Yıldız Z. Multipl skleroz hastalarında apraksi değerlendirme ölçeği apraxia screen of tulia (AST) Türkçe versiyonunun geçerlik ve güvenirliği. [Yüksek Lisans Tezi]. İstanbul: İstanbul Okan Üniversitesi Sağlık Bilimleri Enstitüsü; 2019.
  15. Buchmann I, Dangel M, Finkel L, Jung R, Makhkamova I, Binder A, et al. Limb apraxia profiles in different clinical samples. Clin Neuropsychol 2020;34:217-42. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2019.1585575.
  16. Johnen A, Frommeyer J, Modes F, Wiendl H, Duning T, Lohmann H. Dementia Apraxia Test (DATE): A brief tool to differentiate behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer's dementia based on apraxia profiles. J Alzheimers Dis 2016;49:593-605. doi: 10.3233/ JAD-150447.
  17. Lesourd M, Le Gall D, Baumard J, Croisile B, Jarry C, Osiurak F. Apraxia and Alzheimer's disease: Review and perspectives. Neuropsychol Rev 2013;23:234-56. doi: 10.1007/s11065-013-9235-4.
  18. Apostolova LG. Alzheimer disease. Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2016;22:419-34. doi: 10.1212/ CON.0000000000000307.
  19. Can H, Karakaş S. Bilişsel süreçlerde Alzheimer tipi demansa bağlı değişiklikler. Klinik Psikiyatri 2005;8:37-47.
  20. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:263-9. doi: 10.1016/j. jalz.2011.03.005.
  21. Johnen A, Reul S, Wiendl H, Meuth SG, Duning T. Apraxia profiles-A single cognitive marker to discriminate all variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2018;10:363-71. doi: 10.1016/j. dadm.2018.04.002.
  22. Smits LL, Flapper M, Sistermans N, Pijnenburg YA, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM. Apraxia in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: Validity and reliability of the Van Heugten test for apraxia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2014;38:55-64. doi: 10.1159/000358168.
  23. Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: A concept in evolution. J Intern Med 2014;275:214-28. doi: 10.1111/ joim.12190.
  24. van Heugten CM, Dekker J, Deelman BG, StehmannSaris FC, Kinebanian A. A diagnostic test for apraxia in stroke patients: Internal consistency and diagnostic value. Clin Neuropsychol 1999;13:182-92. doi: 10.1076/ clin.13.2.182.1966.
  25. Ozkan S, Adapinar DO, Elmaci NT, Arslantas D. Apraxia for differentiating Alzheimer's disease from subcortical vascular dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2013;9:947-51. doi: 10.2147/ NDT.S47879.
  26. Ward M, Cecato JF, Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE. Assessment for apraxia in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Dement Neuropsychol 2015;9:71-5. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642015DN91000011.
  27. Crutch SJ, Rossor MN, Warrington EK. The quantitative assessment of apraxic deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 2007;43:976-86. doi: 10.1016/s0010- 9452(08)70695-6.
  28. Lesourd M, Le Gall D, Baumard J, Croisile B, Jarry C, Osiurak F. Apraxia and Alzheimer's disease: Review and perspectives. Neuropsychol Rev 2013;23:234-56. doi: 10.1007/s11065-013-9235-4.
  29. Peigneux P, Salmon E, Garraux G, Laureys S, Willems S, Dujardin K, et al. Neural and cognitive bases of upper limb apraxia in corticobasal degeneration. Neurology 2001;57:1259-68. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.7.1259.
  30. Zwinkels A, Geusgens C, van de Sande P, Van Heugten C. Assessment of apraxia: Inter-rater reliability of a new apraxia test, association between apraxia and other cognitive deficits and prevalence of apraxia in a rehabilitation setting. Clin Rehabil 2004;18:819-27. doi: 10.1191/0269215504cr816oa.
  31. Johnen A, Brandstetter L, Kärgel C, Wiendl H, Lohmann H, Duning T. Shared neural correlates of limb apraxia in early stages of Alzheimer's dementia and behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex 2016;84:1-14. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.08.009.
  32. Cotelli M, Manenti R, Brambilla M, Balconi M. Limb apraxia and verb processing in Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2014;36:843-53. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.948389.